In your own words briefly describe Piaget’s stages of cognitive development and Erikson’s theory of psychosocial development.

NEWS FEATURE

News Feature: The neuroscience of poverty
Neuroscientists are investigating whether growing up poor shapes childrens brains in ways that might also shape their lives.
Alla Katsnelson
Science Writer

It wasnt the birth of her daughter that got cognitive neuroscientist Martha Farah inter- ested in early brain development, but rather the babysitters she hired soon afterward.

Most of these women were also mothers, sin-gle, and struggling to make ends meet with a combination of government benefits and cash from domestic work. Farah found herself get-ting closely involved in their lives: sharing meals, tutoring their children, lending money to their relatives. And she couldnt help but
notice that as time went on, her child ended up on a different track from theirs.These kids started life with the same evi-dent potential as my own daughter: loving
their moms, learning words, playing games,asking questions,says Farah, who directs the Center for Neuroscience and Society at the University of Pennsylvania. But somehow they found their ways onto a different kind of life trajectory: toward lower achievement and fewer options in life.The observations ate at her, so she started to
investigate. The literature revealed plenty of social science research showing a predictive link between childrens socioeconomic status (SES) and lifelong health outcomes, academic achievement, and mental health. But nobody had ever made an explicit link to brain devel-opment. Farah began to wonder, could poverty be shaping these childrens entire lives by shaping their brains in ways that diminish their chances of ever escaping poverty? That was about 15 years ago, and from the start, sociologists, educational psychologists, and economists voiced enthusiasm about the idea of extending findings from the social science realm to the contours of the brain. Ironically, though, says Farah, her neuroscientist colleagues were, on the whole, less excited with her newfound research question. I got grant reviews saying
Youre equating poverty with a brain disease,or Youre pathologizing poor childrenthis is irresponsible research,’”she recalls.
In the decade since Farah s team published their first paper on the topic (1), however, neu- roscientists and cognitive psychologists have begun to dive into the fray. Half a dozen stud-ies have correlated family SES with hippocam-pal volume in childhood; a handful have also pointed to differences in other brain structures and differences in the trajectory of brain growth. Recently, studies have started linking such brain differences to real-world outcomes like academic test scores. Even so, what these
early data actually mean is still in question,  and most agree the field is still in its infancy.
Concerns remain, too, about the broader  significance of the research. If growing up
in poverty leaves its mark on the brain in childhood, how reversible is it? Whats the un- derlying cause? And, critics ask, if a social pro-gram has already been shown to alter the pathsof poor families for the better, is the neurosci-ence really necessary to know that it works?I think we re onto something here; I do think that poverty is affecting childrens brain  development,says Seth Pollak, a professor of psychology at the University of Wisconsin, Madison. But I think we have to be very cautious, particularly because this is an area of science that is right on the edge of being able to have some policy implications.

Visible Differences
The United States has some of the highest levels of childhood poverty outside the de-
veloping world, with one in five of all chil-dren15 million in total living below the federal poverty line of $24,250 for a family of four (2, 3). Economic deprivation isnt simply  the absence of money, Farah says. For poor kids, it goes hand in hand with differences from other children in nutrition and prenatal
The major foci in the brain that appear to show disparities in poor children are the hip-pocampus and frontal lobe. These 3D renderings depict the hippocampus in blue and the frontal lobe in red/yellow. Image courtesy of Jamie Hanson (Duke University, Durham, NC,and the University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Chapel Hill, NC).
1553015532 | PNAS | December 22, 2015 | vol. 112 | no. 51 www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.1522683112 care, parental education levels, neighborhood,

and other environmental stressors. One of the most striking effects is the achievement gap:
On the whole, children from poor families score lower on standardized tests, get worse grades, and attend college in much smaller numbers than those in middle class or affluent families. And this discrepancy is growing (4).
By bringing in neuroscience you get a whole bunch of new potential explanations
for the effects of poverty on the child,says Farah. For example, she says, poor children tend to have worse memories than their more affluent peers, in part because of higher levels of stress in poor families. Neuroscience reveals why: One design quirk of the brain is that the hippocampus, a key structure for consolidat- ing memories, happens to be loaded with stress hormone receptors.
The early data revealed some intriguing disparities. Farah and her colleaguesincluding her then-graduate student Kimberly Noble, now a professor of neuroscience and education at Columbia Universitys Teachers College, and
University of Pennsylvania neonatologist Hallam Hurtfound that SES didnt affect
cognition across the board. Rather, deficits clustered in functions thought to engage specific brain circuits: for example, language, certain dimensions of memory, and the ability to regulate thoughts and emotions (1, 5). Early-language expert Patricia Kuhl at the University of Washington used functional MRI to conclude that low-SES five-year-olds showed less specialization in a key region of the cortex implicated in reading (6). Helen Nevilles group at the University of Oregon measured brain responses, called event-related potentials, to conclude that low-SES three- to eight-year-olds are slower to pay attention to a specified auditory input (7).

Much of the work over the past few years,though, has highlighted structural differences, including preliminary findings in the amygdala,which plays a role in processing fear and other emotions, and in the prefrontal cortex, involved in decision-making and self-control. The most consistent finding has been that of a smaller hippocampus in low-SES children.
The first study to report a hippocampal size difference came from Pollak s laboratory and relied on existing brain MRIs and family de- mographic data from a group of 317 children aged 418 years, drawn from a national United States developmental database (8). Two years later, Joan Luby and her colleagues at the University of Washington also found signifi- cantly smaller hippocampi in 145 poor 6- to 12-year-olds followed since preschool-age and compared with kids not living in poverty (9).
Lubys team hadn t even set out to explore  how poverty changes the brain; they tracked it just to account for that variable in their study of depression and other psychiatric problems.
But when they crunched the numbers, the signal was huge, Luby says. Even though it wasnt our primary agenda, the data insisted that we follow up.The researchers also asked children and parents about stressful life events and assessed how encouraging and supportive their mothers were in a laboratory task. A statistical analysis found that the effects of poverty on the brain were stronger in children whose mothers were less nurturing or who experienced stress at home. The largest analysis of brain structural dif- ferences across socioeconomic lines came this past March. Examining MRI scans of more than 1,000 subjects between the ages of 3 and 20 years from a national database, Noble et al.
(10) detected smaller hippocampal volume in kids from families with less education (an oft-used proxy for SES). More significantly, they found differences in the surface area of the cerebral cortex. During childhood and adoles-cence, as myelin forms and neurons find their proper connections, cortical thickness de- creases and surface area increases; past studies have associated the resulting surface area changes with intelligence. In Nobles study, on average, every additional year of parental edu- cation was associated with an increase of cor- tical surface area, specifically in parts of the cortex that handle language, reading, and self- regulation. The effect tracked with income as well, especially for the poorest families (10).
Complex Implications
One benefit of studying measurable changes in brain structure, says Farah, is that unlike functional imaging, it doesnt require fore- knowledge of which cognitive processes might be affected. However, structural studies have their own challenges of interpretation. The hippocampal disparity, although robust across several studies in children, is not consistently seen in adults. Does that mean that kids brains catch up? Its also unclear when exactly these differences emerge. Pollak and his col- leagues compared the rate of overall brain growth in low-SES versus middle class kids and found that the trajectory starts to fall off for poor children in the toddler years, with a  clear difference by the age of four (11). But in an ongoing longitudinal study of extremely poor children, Farah and her colleagues are finding differences in cortical gray matter volume within the first couple months of life, suggesting that some poverty-associated brain changes could be occurring prenatally.
Given the complexities of human brain development, not to mention the interplay of genes and environment during that process,its still quite difficult to know what to make of the structural observations. Pollak and others also stress that these results are population averages, which smooth over a lot of variation.
You cant look at an individual child from a low-SES background and glean much about her brain. Understanding the long-term envi-ronmental effects and whether or how kidsdeveloping brains compensate for them will require more nuanced experiments that ac-  count for individual differences in childrens response to poverty, says Silvia Bunge, a pro- fessor of psychology and neuroscience at the University of California, Berkeley. These could include protective cultural or personal traits that foster what psychologists term resilience.
Slowly, the field is edging toward making more complex connections. Earlier this year, two published studies explicitly linked structural differences in the brains of children from disadvantaged families to their achievement.
A small study of 58 adolescents by John Gabrielis group at the Massachusetts In- stitute of Technology, published in April, was the first to relate cortical volume to scores on
As age increases, household SES correlates with gray matter volume, according to work by Hanson et al. (11). Reproduced with permission from ref. 11.
Katsnelson PNAS | December 22, 2015 | vol. 112 | no. 51 | 15531
NEWSFEATURE

Journal Reflection 4:
Answer all 3 parts of the question for full credit.

(1) In your own words briefly describe Piaget’s stages of cognitive development and Erikson’s theory of psychosocial development.

(2) Why do you think it is important for social workers to understand cognitive and
socioemotional development?

(3) Describe one or two scenarios when, as a social worker, it would be helpful to explain these stages of development to caregivers.

In your own words briefly describe Piaget’s stages of cognitive development and Erikson’s theory of psychosocial development.
Scroll to top